The Origin of
Species
Chapter 1: Variation Under Domestication
by Charles Darwin
Chapter 1: Variation Under Domestication
by Charles Darwin
Causes of Variability - Effects of
Habit - Correlation of Growth - Inheritance - Character of Domestic Varieties
- Difficulty of distinguishing between Varieties and Species - Origin of
Domestic Varieties from one or more Species - Domestic pigeons, their
Differences and Origin - Principle of Selection anciently followed, its
Effects - Methodical and Unconscious Selection - Unknown Origin of our
Domestic Productions - Circumstances favourable to Man's power of Selection
|
When we look to the
individuals of the same variety or sub-variety of our older cultivated plants
and animals, one of the first points which strikes us, is, that they generally
differ much more from each other, than do the individuals of any one species or
variety in a state of nature. When we reflect on the vast diversity of the
plants and animals which have been cultivated, and which have varied during all
ages under the most different climates and treatment, I think we are driven to
conclude that this greater variability is simply due to our domestic
productions having been raised under conditions of life not so uniform as, and
somewhat different from, those to which the parent-species have been exposed
under nature. There is, also, I think, some probability in the view propounded
by Andrew Knight, that this variability may be partly connected with excess of
food. It seems pretty clear that organic beings must be exposed during several
generations to the new conditions of life to cause any appreciable amount of
variation; and that when the organisation has once begun to vary, it generally
continues to vary for many generations. No case is on record of a variable
being ceasing to be variable under cultivation. Our oldest cultivated plants,
such as wheat, still often yield new varieties: our oldest domesticated animals
are still capable of rapid improvement or modification.
It has been disputed at what period of
time the causes of variability, whatever they may be, generally act; whether
during the early or late period of development of the embryo, or at the instant
of conception. Geoffroy St Hilaire's experiments show that unnatural treatment
of the embryo causes monstrosities; and monstrosities cannot be separated by
any clear line of distinction from mere variations. But I am strongly inclined
to suspect that the most frequent cause of variability may be attributed to the
male and female reproductive elements having been affected prior to the act of
conception. Several reasons make me believe in this; but the chief one is the
remarkable effect which confinement or cultivation has on the functions of the
reproductive system; this system appearing to be far more susceptible than any
other part of the organization, to the action of any change in the conditions
of life. Nothing is more easy than to tame an animal, and few things more
difficult than to get it to breed freely under confinement, even in the many
cases when the male and female unite. How many animals there are which will not
breed, though living long under not very close confinement in their native
country! This is generally attributed to vitiated instincts; but how many
cultivated plants display the utmost vigour, and yet rarely or never seed! In some
few such cases it has been found out that very trifling changes, such as a
little more or less water at some particular period of growth, will determine
whether or not the plant sets a seed. I cannot here enter on the copious
details which I have collected on this curious subject; but to show how
singular the laws are which determine the reproduction of animals under
confinement, I may just mention that carnivorous animals, even from the
tropics, breed in this country pretty freely under confinement, with the
exception of the plantigrades or bear family; whereas, carnivorous birds, with
the rarest exceptions, hardly ever lay fertile eggs. Many exotic plants have
pollen utterly worthless, in the same exact condition as in the most sterile
hybrids. When, on the one hand, we see domesticated animals and plants, though
often weak and sickly, yet breeding quite freely under confinement; and when,
on the other hand, we see individuals, though taken young from a state of
nature, perfectly tamed, long-lived, and healthy (of which I could give
numerous instances), yet having their reproductive system so seriously affected
by unperceived causes as to fail in acting, we need not be surprised at this
system, when it does act under confinement, acting not quite regularly, and
producing offspring not perfectly like their parents or variable.
Sterility has been said to be the bane of
horticulture; but on this view we owe variability to the same cause which
produces sterility; and variability is the source of all the choicest
productions of the garden. I may add, that as some organisms will breed most
freely under the most unnatural conditions (for instance, the rabbit and ferret
kept in hutches), showing that their reproductive system has not been thus
affected; so will some animals and plants withstand domestication or
cultivation, and vary very slightly perhaps hardly more than in a state of
nature.
A long list could easily be given of
'sporting plants;' by this term gardeners mean a single bud or offset, which
suddenly assumes a new and sometimes very different character from that of the
rest of the plant. Such buds can be propagated by grafting, &c., and
sometimes by seed. These 'sports' are extremely rare under nature, but far from
rare under cultivation; and in this case we see that the treatment of the
parent has affected a bud or offset, and not the ovules or pollen. But it is
the opinion of most physiologists that there is no essential difference between
a bud and an ovule in their earliest stages of formation; so that, in
fact,'sports' support my view, that variability may be largely attributed to
the ovules or pollen, or to both, having been affected by the treatment of the
parent prior to the act of conception. These cases anyhow show that variation
is not necessarily connected, as some authors have supposed, with the act of
generation.
Seedlings from the same fruit, and the
young of the same litter, sometimes differ considerably from each other, though
both the young and the parents, as Muller has remarked, have apparently been
exposed to exactly the same conditions of life; and this shows how unimportant
the direct effects of the conditions of life are in comparison with the laws of
reproduction, and of growth, and of inheritance; for had the action of the
conditions been direct, if any of the young had varied, all would probably have
varied in the same manner. To judge how much, in the case of any variation, we
should attribute to the direct action of heat, moisture, light, food, &c.,
is most difficult: my impression is, that with animals such agencies have
produced very little direct effect, though apparently more in the case of
plants. Under this point of view, Mr Buckman's recent experiments on plants
seem extremely valuable. When all or nearly all the individuals exposed to
certain conditions are affected in the same way, the change at first appears to
be directly due to such conditions; but in some cases it can be shown that
quite opposite conditions produce similar changes of structure. Nevertheless
some slight amount of change may, I think, be attributed to the direct action
of the conditions of life as, in some cases, increased size from amount of
food, colour from particular kinds of food and from light, and perhaps the
thickness of fur from climate.
Habit also has a deciding influence, as in
the period of flowering with plants when transported from one climate to
another. In animals it has a more marked effect; for instance, I find in the
domestic duck that the bones of the wing weigh less and the bones of the leg
more, in proportion to the whole skeleton, than do the same bones in the
wild-duck; and I presume that this change may be safely attributed to the
domestic duck flying much less, and walking more, than its wild parent. The
great and inherited development of the udders in cows and goats in countries
where they are habitually milked, in comparison with the state of these organs
in other countries, is another instance of the effect of use. Not a single
domestic animal can be named which has not in some country drooping ears; and
the view suggested by some authors, that the drooping is due to the disuse of
the muscles of the ear, from the animals not being much alarmed by danger,
seems probable.
There are many laws regulating variation,
some few of which can be dimly seen, and will be hereafter briefly mentioned. I
will here only allude to what may be called correlation of growth. Any change
in the embryo or larva will almost certainly entail changes in the mature
animal. In monstrosities, the correlations between quite distinct parts are
very curious; and many instances are given in Isidore Geoffroy St Hilaire's
great work on this subject. Breeders believe that long limbs are almost always
accompanied by an elongated head. Some instances of correlation are quite
whimsical; thus cats with blue eyes are invariably deaf; colour and
constitutional peculiarities go together, of which many remarkable cases could
be given amongst animals and plants. From the facts collected by Heusinger, it
appears that white sheep and pigs are differently affected from coloured
individuals by certain vegetable poisons. Hairless dogs have imperfect teeth;
long-haired and coarse-haired animals are apt to have, as is asserted, long or
many horns; pigeons with feathered feet have skin between their outer toes;
pigeons with short beaks have small feet, and those with long beaks large feet.
Hence, if man goes on selecting, and thus augmenting, any peculiarity, he will
almost certainly unconsciously modify other parts of the structure, owing to
the mysterious laws of the correlation of growth.
The result of the various, quite unknown,
or dimly seen laws of variation is infinitely complex and diversified. It is
well worth while carefully to study the several treatises published on some of
our old cultivated plants, as on the hyacinth, potato, even the dahlia,
&c.; and it is really surprising to note the endless points in structure
and constitution in which the varieties and sub varieties differ slightly from
each other. The whole organization seems to have become plastic, and tends to
depart in some small degree from that of the parental type.
Any variation which is not inherited is
unimportant for us. But the number and diversity of inheritable deviations of
structure, both those of slight and those of considerable physiological
importance, is endless. Dr Prosper Lucas's treatise, in two large volumes, is
the fullest and the best on this subject. No breeder doubts how strong is the
tendency to inheritance: like produces like is his fundamental belief: doubts
have been thrown on this principle by theoretical writers alone. When a
deviation appears not unfrequently, and we see it in the father and child, we
cannot tell whether it may not be due to the same original cause acting on
both; but when amongst individuals, apparently exposed to the same conditions,
any very rare deviation, due to some extraordinary combination of
circumstances, appears in the parent say, once amongst several million
individuals and it reappears in the child, the mere doctrine of chances almost
compels us to attribute its reappearance to inheritance. Every one must have
heard of cases of albinism, prickly skin, hairy bodies, &c. appearing in
several members of the same family. If strange and rare deviations of structure
are truly inherited, less strange and commoner deviations may be freely
admitted to be inheritable. Perhaps the correct way of viewing the whole
subject, would be, to look at the inheritance of every character whatever as
the rule, and non-inheritance as the anomaly.
The laws governing inheritance are quite
unknown; no one can say why the same peculiarity in different individuals of
the same species, and in individuals of different species, is sometimes
inherited and sometimes not so; why the child often reverts in certain
characters to its grandfather or grandmother or other much more remote
ancestor; why a peculiarity is often transmitted from one sex to both sexes or
to one sex alone, more commonly but not exclusively to the like sex. It is a
fact of some little importance to us, that peculiarities appearing in the males
of our domestic breeds are often transmitted either exclusively, or in a much
greater degree, to males alone. A much more important rule, which I think may
be trusted, is that, at whatever period of life a peculiarity first appears, it
tends to appear in the offspring at a corresponding age, though sometimes
earlier. In many cases this could not be otherwise: thus the inherited
peculiarities in the horns of cattle could appear only in the offspring when
nearly mature; peculiarities in the silkworm are known to appear at the
corresponding caterpillar or cocoon stage. But hereditary diseases and some
other facts make me believe that the rule has a wider extension, and that when
there is no apparent reason why a peculiarity should appear at any particular
age, yet that it does tend to appear in the offspring at the same period at
which it first appeared in the parent. I believe this rule to be of the highest
importance in explaining the laws of embryology. These remarks are of course
confined to the first appearance of the peculiarity, and not
to its primary cause, which may have acted on the ovules or male element; in
nearly the same manner as in the crossed offspring from a short-horned cow by a
long-horned bull, the greater length of horn, though appearing late in life, is
clearly due to the male element.
Having alluded to the subject of
reversion, I may here refer to a statement often made by naturalists namely,
that our domestic varieties, when run wild, gradually but certainly revert in
character to their aboriginal stocks. Hence it has been argued that no
deductions can be drawn from domestic races to species in a state of nature. I
have in vain endeavoured to discover on what decisive facts the above statement
has so often and so boldly been made. There would be great difficulty in
proving its truth: we may safely conclude that very many of the most
strongly-marked domestic varieties could not possibly live in a wild state. In
many cases we do not know what the aboriginal stock was, and so could not tell
whether or not nearly perfect reversion had ensued. It would be quite
necessary, in order to prevent the effects of intercrossing, that only a single
variety should be turned loose in its new home. Nevertheless, as our varieties
certainly do occasionally revert in some of their characters to ancestral
forms, it seems to me not improbable, that if we could succeed in naturalising,
or were to cultivate, during many generations, the several races, for instance,
of the cabbage, in very poor soil (in which case, however, some effect would
have to be attributed to the direct action of the poor soil), that they would
to a large extent, or even wholly, revert to the wild aboriginal stock. Whether
or not the experiment would succeed, is not of great importance for our line of
argument; for by the experiment itself the conditions of life are changed. If
it could be shown that our domestic varieties manifested a strong tendency to
reversion, that is, to lose their acquired characters, whilst kept under
unchanged conditions, and whilst kept in a considerable body, so that free
intercrossing might check, by blending together, any slight deviations of
structure, in such case, I grant that we could deduce nothing from domestic
varieties in regard to species. But there is not a shadow of evidence in favour
of this view: to assert that we could not breed our cart and race-horses, long
and short-horned cattle and poultry of various breeds, and esculent vegetables,
for an almost infinite number of generations, would be opposed to all
experience. I may add, that when under nature the conditions of life do change,
variations and reversions of character probably do occur; but natural
selection, as will hereafter be explained, will determine how far the new
characters thus arising shall be preserved.
When we look to the hereditary varieties
or races of our domestic animals and plants, and compare them with species
closely allied together, we generally perceive in each domestic race, as
already remarked, less uniformity of character than in true species. Domestic
races of the same species, also, often have a somewhat monstrous character; by
which I mean, that, although differing from each other, and from the other
species of the same genus, in several trifling respects, they often differ in
an extreme degree in some one part, both when compared one with another, and
more especially when compared with all the species in nature to which they are
nearest allied. With these exceptions (and with that of the perfect fertility
of varieties when crossed, a subject hereafter to be discussed), domestic races
of the same species differ from each other in the same manner as, only in most
cases in a lesser degree than, do closely-allied species of the same genus in a
state of nature. I think this must be admitted, when we find that there are
hardly any domestic races, either amongst animals or plants, which have not
been ranked by some competent judges as mere varieties, and by other competent
judges as the descendants of aboriginally distinct species. If any marked distinction
existed between domestic races and species, this source of doubt could not so
perpetually recur. It has often been stated that domestic races do not differ
from each other in characters of generic value. I think it could be shown that
this statement is hardly correct; but naturalists differ most widely in
determining what characters are of generic value; all such valuations being at
present empirical. Moreover, on the view of the origin of genera which I shall
presently give, we have no right to expect often to meet with generic
differences in our domesticated productions.
When we attempt to estimate the amount of
structural difference between the domestic races of the same species, we are
soon involved in doubt, from not knowing whether they have descended from one
or several parent-species. This point, if could be cleared up, would be
interesting; if, for instance, it could be shown that the greyhound,
bloodhound, terrier, spaniel, and bull-dog, which we all know propagate their
kind so truly, were the offspring of any single species, then such facts would
have great weight in making us doubt about the immutability of the many very
closely allied and natural species for instance, of the many foxes inhabiting
different quarters of the world. I do not believe, as we shall presently see,
that all our dogs have descended from any one wild species; but, in the case of
some other domestic races, there is presumptive, or even strong, evidence in
favour of this view.
It has often been assumed that man has chosen
for domestication animals and plants having an extraordinary inherent tendency
to vary, and likewise to withstand diverse climates. I do not dispute that
these capacities have added largely to the value of most of our domesticated
productions; but how could a savage possibly know, when he first tamed an
animal, whether it would vary in succeeding generations, and whether it would
endure other climates? Has the little variability of the ass or guinea-fowl, or
the small power of endurance of warmth by the reindeer, or of cold by the
common camel, prevented their domestication? I cannot doubt that if other
animals and plants, equal in number to our domesticated productions, and
belonging to equally diverse classes and countries, were taken from a state of nature,
and could be made to breed for an equal number of generations under
domestication, they would vary on an average as largely as the parent species
of our existing domesticated productions have varied.
In the case of most of our anciently
domesticated animals and plants, I do not think it is possible to come to any
definite conclusion, whether they have descended from one or several species.
The argument mainly relied on by those who believe in the multiple origin of
our domestic animals is, that we find in the most ancient records, more
especially on the monuments of Egypt, much diversity in the breeds; and that
some of the breeds closely resemble, perhaps are identical with, those still
existing. Even if this latter fact were found more strictly and generally true
than seems to me to be the case, what does it show, but that some of our breeds
originated there, four or five thousand years ago? But Mr Horner's researches
have rendered it in some degree probable that man sufficiently civilized to
have manufactured pottery existed in the valley of the Nile thirteen or
fourteen thousand years ago; and who will pretend to say how long before these
ancient periods, savages, like those of Tierra del Fuego or Australia, who
possess a semi-domestic dog, may not have existed in Egypt?
The whole subject must, I think, remain
vague; nevertheless, I may, without here entering on any details, state that,
from geographical and other considerations, I think it highly probable that our
domestic dogs have descended from several wild species. In regard to sheep and
goats I can form no opinion. I should think, from facts communicated to me by
Mr Blyth, on the habits, voice, and constitution, &c., of the humped Indian
cattle, that these had descended from a different aboriginal stock from our
European cattle; and several competent judges believe that these latter have
had more than one wild parent. With respect to horses, from reasons which I
cannot give here, I am doubtfully inclined to believe, in opposition to several
authors, that all the races have descended from one wild stock. Mr Blyth, whose
opinion, from his large and varied stores of knowledge, I should value more
than that of almost any one, thinks that all the breeds of poultry have
proceeded from the common wild Indian fowl (Gallus bankiva). In regard to ducks
and rabbits, the breeds of which differ considerably from each other in
structure, I do not doubt that they all have descended from the common wild
duck and rabbit.
The doctrine of the origin of our several
domestic races from several aboriginal stocks, has been carried to an absurd
extreme by some authors. They believe that every race which breeds true, let
the distinctive characters be ever so slight, has had its wild prototype. At
this rate there must have existed at least a score of species of wild cattle,
as many sheep, and several goats in Europe alone, and several even within Great
Britain. One author believes that there formerly existed in Great Britain
eleven wild species of sheep peculiar to it! When we bear in mind that Britain
has now hardly one peculiar mammal, and France but few distinct from those of
Germany and conversely, and so with Hungary, Spain, &c., but that each of
these kingdoms possesses several peculiar breeds of cattle, sheep, &c., we
must admit that many domestic breeds have originated in Europe; for whence
could they have been derived, as these several countries do not possess a
number of peculiar species as distinct parent-stocks? So it is in India. Even
in the case of the domestic dogs of the whole world, which I fully admit have
probably descended from several wild species, I cannot doubt that there has
been an immense amount of inherited variation. Who can believe that animals
closely resembling the Italian greyhound, the bloodhound, the bull-dog, or
Blenheim spaniel, &c. so unlike all wild Canidae ever existed freely in a
state of nature? It has often been loosely said that all our races of dogs have
been produced by the crossing of a few aboriginal species; but by crossing we
can get only forms in some degree intermediate between their parents; and if we
account for our several domestic races by this process, we must admit the
former existence of the most extreme forms, as the Italian greyhound,
bloodhound, bull-dog, &c., in the wild state. Moreover, the possibility of
making distinct races by crossing has been greatly exaggerated. There can be no
doubt that a race may be modified by occasional crosses, if aided by the
careful selection of those individual mongrels, which present any desired
character; but that a race could be obtained nearly intermediate between two
extremely different races or species, I can hardly believe. Sir J. Sebright
expressly experimentised for this object, and failed. The offspring from the
first cross between two pure breeds is tolerably and sometimes (as I have found
with pigeons) extremely uniform, and everything seems simple enough; but when
these mongrels are crossed one with another for several generations, hardly two
of them will be alike, and then the extreme difficulty, or rather utter
hopelessness, of the task becomes apparent. Certainly, a breed intermediate
between two very distinct breeds could not be got without
extreme care and long-continued selection; nor can I find a single case on
record of a permanent race having been thus formed.
On the Breeds of the
Domestic pigeon.
Believing that it is always best to study
some special group, I have, after deliberation, taken up domestic pigeons. I
have kept every breed which I could purchase or obtain, and have been most
kindly favoured with skins from several quarters of the world, more especially
by the Hon. W. Elliot from India, and by the Hon. C. Murray from Persia. Many
treatises in different languages have been published on pigeons, and some of them
are very important, as being of considerably antiquity. I have associated with
several eminent fanciers, and have been permitted to join two of the London
Pigeon Clubs. The diversity of the breeds is something astonishing. Compare the
English carrier and the short-faced tumbler, and see the wonderful difference
in their beaks, entailing corresponding differences in their skulls. The
carrier, more especially the male bird, is also remarkable from the wonderful
development of the carunculated skin about the head, and this is accompanied by
greatly elongated eyelids, very large external orifices to the nostrils, and a
wide gape of mouth. The short-faced tumbler has a beak in outline almost like
that of a finch; and the common tumbler has the singular and strictly inherited
habit of flying at a great height in a compact flock, and tumbling in the air
head over heels. The runt is a bird of great size, with long, massive beak and
large feet; some of the sub-breeds of runts have very long necks, others very
long wings and tails, others singularly short tails. The barb is allied to the
carrier, but, instead of a very long beak, has a very short and very broad one.
The pouter has a much elongated body, wings, and legs; and its enormously
developed crop, which it glories in inflating, may well excite astonishment and
even laughter. The turbit has a very short and conical beak, with a line of
reversed feathers down the breast; and it has the habit of continually
expanding slightly the upper part of the oesophagus. The Jacobin has the
feathers so much reversed along the back of the neck that they form a hood, and
it has, proportionally to its size, much elongated wing and tail feathers. The
trumpeter and laugher, as their names express, utter a very different coo from
the other breeds. The fantail has thirty or even forty tail-feathers, instead
of twelve or fourteen, the normal number in all members of the great pigeon
family; and these feathers are kept expanded, and are carried so erect that in
good birds the head and tail touch; the oil-gland is quite aborted. Several
other less distinct breeds might have been specified.
In the skeletons of the several breeds,
the development of the bones of the face in length and breadth and curvature
differs enormously. The shape, as well as the breadth and length of the ramus
of the lower jaw, varies in a highly remarkable manner. The number of the
caudal and sacral vertebrae vary; as does the number of the ribs, together with
their relative breadth and the presence of processes. The size and shape of the
apertures in the sternum are highly variable; so is the degree of divergence
and relative size of the two arms of the furcula. The proportional width of the
gape of mouth, the proportional length of the eyelids, of the orifice of the nostrils,
of the tongue (not always in strict correlation with the length of beak), the
size of the crop and of the upper part of the oesophagus; the development and
abortion of the oil-gland; the number of the primary wing and caudal feathers;
the relative length of wing and tail to each other and to the body; the
relative length of leg and of the feet; the number of scutellae on the toes,
the development of skin between the toes, are all points of structure which are
variable. The period at which the perfect plumage is acquired varies, as does
the state of the down with which the nestling birds are clothed when hatched.
The shape and size of the eggs vary. The manner of flight differs remarkably;
as does in some breeds the voice and disposition. Lastly, in certain breeds,
the males and females have come to differ to a slight degree from each other.
Altogether at least a score of pigeons
might be chosen, which if shown to an ornithologist, and he were told that they
were wild birds, would certainly, I think, be ranked by him as well-defined
species. Moreover, I do not believe that any ornithologist would place the
English carrier, the short-faced tumbler, the runt, the barb, pouter, and
fantail in the same genus; more especially as in each of these breeds several
truly-inherited sub-breeds, or species as he might have called them, could be
shown him.
Great as the differences are between the
breeds of pigeons, I am fully convinced that the common opinion of naturalists
is correct, namely, that all have descended from the rock-pigeon (Columba
livia), including under this term several geographical races or sub-species,
which differ from each other in the most trifling respects. As several of the
reasons which have led me to this belief are in some degree applicable in other
cases, I will here briefly give them. If the several breeds are not varieties,
and have not proceeded from the rock-pigeon, they must have descended from at
least seven or eight aboriginal stocks; for it is impossible to make the
present domestic breeds by the crossing of any lesser number: how, for
instance, could a pouter be produced by crossing two breeds unless one of the
parent-stocks possessed the characteristic enormous crop? The supposed
aboriginal stocks must all have been rock-pigeons, that is, not breeding or
willingly perching on trees. But besides C. livia, with its geographical
sub-species, only two or three other species of rock-pigeons are known; and
these have not any of the characters of the domestic breeds. Hence the supposed
aboriginal stocks must either still exist in the countries where they were
originally domesticated, and yet be unknown to ornithologists; and this,
considering their size, habits, and remarkable characters, seems very
improbable; or they must have become extinct in the wild state. But birds
breeding on precipices, and good fliers, are unlikely to be exterminated; and
the common rock-pigeon, which has the same habits with the domestic breeds, has
not been exterminated even on several of the smaller British islets, or on the
shores of the Mediterranean. Hence the supposed extermination of so many
species having similar habits with the rock-pigeon seems to me a very rash
assumption. Moreover, the several above-named domesticated breeds have been
transported to all parts of the world, and, therefore, some of them must have
been carried back again into their native country; but not one has ever become
wild or feral, though the dovecot-pigeon, which is the rock-pigeon in a very
slightly altered state, has become feral in several places. Again, all recent
experience shows that it is most difficult to get any wild animal to breed
freely under domestication; yet on the hypothesis of the multiple origin of our
pigeons, it must be assumed that at least seven or eight species were so
thoroughly domesticated in ancient times by half-civilized man, as to be quite
prolific under confinement.
An argument, as it seems to me, of great
weight, and applicable in several other cases, is, that the above-specified
breeds, though agreeing generally in constitution, habits, voice, colouring,
and in most parts of their structure, with the wild rock-pigeon, yet are
certainly highly abnormal in other parts of their structure: we may look in
vain throughout the whole great family of Columbidae for a beak like that of
the English carrier, or that of the short-faced tumbler, or barb; for reversed
feathers like those of the jacobin; for a crop like that of the pouter; for
tail-feathers like those of the fantail. Hence it must be assumed not only that
half-civilized man succeeded in thoroughly domesticating several species, but
that he intentionally or by chance picked out extraordinarily abnormal species;
and further, that these very species have since all become extinct or unknown.
So many strange contingencies seem to me improbable in the highest degree.
Some facts in regard to the colouring of
pigeons well deserve consideration. The rock-pigeon is of a slaty-blue, and has
a white rump (the Indian sub-species, C. intermedia of Strickland, having it
bluish); the tail has a terminal dark bar, with the bases of the outer feathers
externally edged with white; the wings have two black bars: some semi-domestic
breeds and some apparently truly wild breeds have, besides the two black bars,
the wings chequered with black. These several marks do not occur together in
any other species of the whole family. Now, in every one of the domestic
breeds, taking thoroughly well-bred birds, all the above marks, even to the
white edging of the outer tail-feathers, sometimes concur perfectly developed.
Moreover, when two birds belonging to two distinct breeds are crossed, neither
of which is blue or has any of the above-specified marks, the mongrel offspring
are very apt suddenly to acquire these characters; for instance, I crossed some
uniformly white fantails with some uniformly black barbs, and they produced
mottled brown and black birds; these I again crossed together, and one
grandchild of the pure white fantail and pure black barb was of as beautiful a
blue colour, with the white rump, double black wing-bar, and barred and
white-edged tail-feathers, as any wild rock-pigeon! We can understand these
facts, on the well-known principle of reversion to ancestral characters, if all
the domestic breeds have descended from the rock-pigeon. But if we deny this,
we must make one of the two following highly improbable suppositions. Either,
firstly, that all the several imagined aboriginal stocks were coloured and
marked like the rock-pigeon, although no other existing species is thus
coloured and marked, so that in each separate breed there might be a tendency
to revert to the very same colours and markings. Or, secondly, that each breed,
even the purest, has within a dozen or, at most, within a score of generations,
been crossed by the rock-pigeon: I say within a dozen or twenty generations,
for we know of no fact countenancing the belief that the child ever reverts to
some one ancestor, removed by a greater number of generations. In a breed which
has been crossed only once with some distinct breed, the tendency to reversion
to any character derived from such cross will naturally become less and less,
as in each succeeding generation there will be less of the foreign blood; but
when there has been no cross with a distinct breed, and there is a tendency in
both parents to revert to a character, which has been lost during some former
generation, this tendency, for all that we can see to the contrary, may be
transmitted undiminished for an indefinite number of generations. These two distinct
cases are often confounded in treatises on inheritance.
Lastly, the hybrids or mongrels from
between all the domestic breeds of pigeons are perfectly fertile. I can state
this from my own observations, purposely made on the most distinct breeds. Now,
it is difficult, perhaps impossible, to bring forward one case of the hybrid
offspring of two animals clearly distinct being themselves
perfectly fertile. Some authors believe that long-continued domestication
eliminates this strong tendency to sterility: from the history of the dog I
think there is some probability in this hypothesis, if applied to species
closely related together, though it is unsupported by a single experiment. But
to extend the hypothesis so far as to suppose that species, aboriginally as
distinct as carriers, tumblers, pouters, and fantails now are, should yield
offspring perfectly fertile, inter se, seems to me rash in the
extreme.
From these several reasons, namely, the
improbability of man having formerly got seven or eight supposed species of
pigeons to breed freely under domestication; these supposed species being quite
unknown in a wild state, and their becoming nowhere feral; these species having
very abnormal characters in certain respects, as compared with all other
Columbidae, though so like in most other respects to the rock-pigeon; the blue
colour and various marks occasionally appearing in all the breeds, both when
kept pure and when crossed; the mongrel offspring being perfectly fertile; from
these several reasons, taken together, I can feel no doubt that all our
domestic breeds have descended from the Columba livia with its geographical
sub-species.
In favour of this view, I may add,
firstly, that C. livia, or the rock-pigeon, has been found capable of
domestication in Europe and in India; and that it agrees in habits and in a
great number of points of structure with all the domestic breeds. Secondly,
although an English carrier or short-faced tumbler differs immensely in certain
characters from the rock-pigeon, yet by comparing the several sub-breeds of
these breeds, more especially those brought from distant countries, we can make
an almost perfect series between the extremes of structure. Thirdly, those
characters which are mainly distinctive of each breed, for instance the wattle
and length of beak of the carrier, the shortness of that of the tumbler, and
the number of tail-feathers in the fantail, are in each breed eminently
variable; and the explanation of this fact will be obvious when we come to
treat of selection. Fourthly, pigeons have been watched, and tended with the
utmost care, and loved by many people. They have been domesticated for
thousands of years in several quarters of the world; the earliest known record
of pigeons is in the fifth Aegyptian dynasty, about 3000 B.C., as was pointed
out to me by Professor Lepsius; but Mr Birch informs me that pigeons are given
in a bill of fare in the previous dynasty. In the time of the Romans, as we
hear from Pliny, immense prices were given for pigeons; 'nay, they are come to
this pass, that they can reckon up their pedigree and race.' Pigeons were much
valued by Akber Khan in India, about the year 1600; never less than 20,000
pigeons were taken with the court. 'The monarchs of Iran and Turan sent him
some very rare birds;' and, continues the courtly historian, 'His Majesty by
crossing the breeds, which method was never practised before, has improved them
astonishingly.' About this same period the Dutch were as eager about pigeons as
were the old Romans. The paramount importance of these considerations in
explaining the immense amount of variation which pigeons have undergone, will
be obvious when we treat of Selection. We shall then, also, see how it is that
the breeds so often have a somewhat monstrous character. It is also a most
favourable circumstance for the production of distinct breeds, that male and
female pigeons can be easily mated for life; and thus different breeds can be
kept together in the same aviary.
I have discussed the probable origin of
domestic pigeons at some, yet quite insufficient, length; because when I first
kept pigeons and watched the several kinds, knowing well how true they bred, I
felt fully as much difficulty in believing that they could ever have descended
from a common parent, as any naturalist could in coming to a similar conclusion
in regard to the many species of finches, or other large groups of birds, in
nature. One circumstance has struck me much; namely, that all the breeders of
the various domestic animals and the cultivators of plants, with whom I have
ever conversed, or whose treatises I have read, are firmly convinced that the
several breeds to which each has attended, are descended from so many
aboriginally distinct species. Ask, as I have asked, a celebrated raiser of
Hereford cattle, whether his cattle might not have descended from long horns,
and he will laugh you to scorn. I have never met a pigeon, or poultry, or duck,
or rabbit fancier, who was not fully convinced that each main breed was
descended from a distinct species. Van Mons, in his treatise on pears and
apples, shows how utterly he disbelieves that the several sorts, for instance a
Ribston-pippin or Codlin-apple, could ever have proceeded from the seeds of the
same tree. Innumerable other examples could be given. The explanation, I think,
is simple: from long-continued study they are strongly impressed with the
differences between the several races; and though they well know that each race
varies slightly, for they win their prizes by selecting such slight
differences, yet they ignore all general arguments, and refuse to sum up in
their minds slight differences accumulated during many successive generations.
May not those naturalists who, knowing far less of the laws of inheritance than
does the breeder, and knowing no more than he does of the intermediate links in
the long lines of descent, yet admit that many of our domestic races have
descended from the same parents may they not learn a lesson of caution, when
they deride the idea of species in a state of nature being lineal descendants
of other species?
Selection
Let us now briefly consider the steps by
which domestic races have been produced, either from one or from several allied
species. Some little effect may, perhaps, be attributed to the direct action of
the external conditions of life, and some little to habit; but he would be a
bold man who would account by such agencies for the differences of a dray and
race horse, a greyhound and bloodhound, a carrier and tumbler pigeon. One of
the most remarkable features in our domesticated races is that we see in them
adaptation, not indeed to the animal's or plant's own good, but to man's use or
fancy. Some variations useful to him have probably arisen suddenly, or by one
step; many botanists, for instance, believe that the fuller's teazle, with its
hooks, which cannot be rivalled by any mechanical contrivance, is only a
variety of the wild Dipsacus; and this amount of change may have suddenly
arisen in a seedling. So it has probably been with the turnspit dog; and this
is known to have been the case with the ancon sheep. But when we compare the
dray-horse and race-horse, the dromedary and camel, the various breeds of sheep
fitted either for cultivated land or mountain pasture, with the wool of one
breed good for one purpose, and that of another breed for another purpose; when
we compare the many breeds of dogs, each good for man in very different ways;
when we compare the gamecock, so pertinacious in battle, with other breeds so
little quarrelsome, with 'everlasting layers' which never desire to sit, and
with the bantam so small and elegant; when we compare the host of agricultural,
culinary, orchard, and flower-garden races of plants, most useful to man at
different seasons and for different purposes, or so beautiful in his eyes, we
must, I think, look further than to mere variability. We cannot suppose that
all the breeds were suddenly produced as perfect and as useful as we now see
them; indeed, in several cases, we know that this has not been their history.
The key is man's power of accumulative selection: nature gives successive
variations; man adds them up in certain directions useful to him. In this sense
he may be said to make for himself useful breeds.
The great power of this principle of
selection is not hypothetical. It is certain that several of our eminent
breeders have, even within a single lifetime, modified to a large extent some
breeds of cattle and sheep. In order fully to realise what they have done, it
is almost necessary to read several of the many treatises devoted to this
subject, and to inspect the animals. Breeders habitually speak of an animal's
organisation as something quite plastic, which they can model almost as they
please. If I had space I could quote numerous passages to this effect from
highly competent authorities. Youatt, who was probably better acquainted with
the works of agriculturalists than almost any other individual, and who was
himself a very good judge of an animal, speaks of the principle of selection as
'that which enables the agriculturist, not only to modify the character of his
flock, but to change it altogether. It is the magician's wand, by means of
which he may summon into life whatever form and mould he pleases.' Lord
Somerville, speaking of what breeders have done for sheep, says: 'It would seem
as if they had chalked out upon a wall a form perfect in itself, and then had
given it existence.' That most skilful breeder, Sir John Sebright, used to say,
with respect to pigeons, that 'he would produce any given feather in three
years, but it would take him six years to obtain head and beak.' In Saxony the
importance of the principle of selection in regard to merino sheep is so fully
recognised, that men follow it as a trade: the sheep are placed on a table and
are studied, like a picture by a connoisseur; this is done three times at
intervals of months, and the sheep are each time marked and classed, so that
the very best may ultimately be selected for breeding.
What English breeders have actually
effected is proved by the enormous prices given for animals with a good
pedigree; and these have now been exported to almost every quarter of the
world. The improvement is by no means generally due to crossing different
breeds; all the best breeders are strongly opposed to this practice, except sometimes
amongst closely allied sub-breeds. And when a cross has been made, the closest
selection is far more indispensable even than in ordinary cases. If selection
consisted merely in separating some very distinct variety, and breeding from
it, the principle would be so obvious as hardly to be worth notice; but its
importance consists in the great effect produced by the accumulation in one
direction, during successive generations, of differences absolutely
inappreciable by an uneducated eye differences which I for one have vainly
attempted to appreciate. Not one man in a thousand has accuracy of eye and
judgement sufficient to become an eminent breeder. If gifted with these
qualities, and he studies his subject for years, and devotes his lifetime to it
with indomitable perseverance, he will succeed, and may make great
improvements; if he wants any of these qualities, he will assuredly fail. Few
would readily believe in the natural capacity and years of practice requisite
to become even a skilful pigeon-fancier.
The same principles are followed by
horticulturists; but the variations are here often more abrupt. No one supposes
that our choicest productions have been produced by a single variation from the
aboriginal stock. We have proofs that this is not so in some cases, in which
exact records have been kept; thus, to give a very trifling instance, the
steadily-increasing size of the common gooseberry may be quoted. We see an
astonishing improvement in many florists' flowers, when the flowers of the
present day are compared with drawings made only twenty or thirty years ago.
When a race of plants is once pretty well established, the seed-raisers do not
pick out the best plants, but merely go over their seed-beds, and pull up the
'rogues,' as they call the plants that deviate from the proper standard. With
animals this kind of selection is, in fact, also followed; for hardly any one
is so careless as to allow his worst animals to breed.
In regard to plants, there is another
means of observing the accumulated effects of selection namely, by comparing
the diversity of flowers in the different varieties of the same species in the
flower-garden; the diversity of leaves, pods, or tubers, or whatever part is
valued, in the kitchen-garden, in comparison with the flowers of the same
varieties; and the diversity of fruit of the same species in the orchard, in
comparison with the leaves and flowers of the same set of varieties. See how
different the leaves of the cabbage are, and how extremely alike the flowers;
how unlike the flowers of the heartsease are, and how alike the leaves; how
much the fruit of the different kinds of gooseberries differ in size, colour,
shape, and hairiness, and yet the flowers present very slight differences. It
is not that the varieties which differ largely in some one point do not differ
at all in other points; this is hardly ever, perhaps never, the case. The laws
of correlation of growth, the importance of which should never be overlooked,
will ensure some differences; but, as a general rule, I cannot doubt that the
continued selection of slight variations, either in the leaves, the flowers, or
the fruit, will produce races differing from each other chiefly in these
characters.
It may be objected that the principle of
selection has been reduced to methodical practice for scarcely more than
three-quarters of a century; it has certainly been more attended to of late
years, and many treatises have been published on the subject; and the result, I
may add, has been, in a corresponding degree, rapid and important. But it is
very far from true that the principle is a modern discovery. I could give
several references to the full acknowledgement of the importance of the
principle in works of high antiquity. In rude and barbarous periods of English
history choice animals were often imported, and laws were passed to prevent
their exportation: the destruction of horses under a certain size was ordered,
and this may be compared to the 'roguing' of plants by nurserymen. The
principle of selection I find distinctly given in an ancient Chinese
encyclopaedia. Explicit rules are laid down by some of the Roman classical
writers. From passages in Genesis, it is clear that the colour of domestic
animals was at that early period attended to. Savages now sometimes cross their
dogs with wild canine animals, to improve the breed, and they formerly did so,
as is attested by passages in Pliny. The savages in South Africa match their
draught cattle by colour, as do some of the Esquimaux their teams of dogs.
Livingstone shows how much good domestic breeds are valued by the negroes of
the interior of Africa who have not associated with Europeans. Some of these
facts do not show actual selection, but they show that the breeding of domestic
animals was carefully attended to in ancient times, and is now attended to by
the lowest savages. It would, indeed, have been a strange fact, had attention
not been paid to breeding, for the inheritance of good and bad qualities is so
obvious.
At the present time, eminent breeders try
by methodical selection, with a distinct object in view, to make a new strain
or sub-breed, superior to anything existing in the country. But, for our
purpose, a kind of Selection, which may be called Unconscious, and which
results from every one trying to possess and breed from the best individual
animals, is more important. Thus, a man who intends keeping pointers naturally
tries to get as good dogs as he can, and afterwards breeds from his own best
dogs, but he has no wish or expectation of permanently altering the breed.
Nevertheless I cannot doubt that this process, continued during centuries,
would improve and modify any breed, in the same way as Bakewell, Collins,
&c., by this very same process, only carried on more methodically, did
greatly modify, even during their own lifetimes, the forms and qualities of
their cattle. Slow and insensible changes of this kind could never be
recognised unless actual measurements or careful drawings of the breeds in
question had been made long ago, which might serve for comparison. In some
cases, however, unchanged or but little changed individuals of the same breed
may be found in less civilised districts, where the breed has been less
improved. There is reason to believe that King Charles's spaniel has been
unconsciously modified to a large extent since the time of that monarch. Some
highly competent authorities are convinced that the setter is directly derived
from the spaniel, and has probably been slowly altered from it. It is known
that the English pointer has been greatly changed within the last century, and
in this case the change has, it is believed, been chiefly effected by crosses
with the fox-hound; but what concerns us is, that the change has been effected
unconsciously and gradually, and yet so effectually, that, though the old
Spanish pointer certainly came from Spain, Mr Barrow has not seen, as I am
informed by him, any native dog in Spain like our pointer.
By a similar process of selection, and by
careful training, the whole body of English racehorses have come to surpass in
fleetness and size the parent Arab stock, so that the latter, by the
regulations for the Goodwood Races, are favoured in the weights they carry.
Lord Spencer and others have shown how the cattle of England have increased in
weight and in early maturity, compared with the stock formerly kept in this
country. By comparing the accounts given in old pigeon treatises of carriers
and tumblers with these breeds as now existing in Britain, India, and Persia,
we can, I think, clearly trace the stages through which they have insensibly
passed, and come to differ so greatly from the rock-pigeon.
Youatt gives an excellent illustration of
the effects of a course of selection, which may be considered as unconsciously
followed, in so far that the breeders could never have expected or even have
wished to have produced the result which ensued namely, the production of two
distinct strains. The two flocks of Leicester sheep kept by Mr Buckley and Mr
Burgess, as Mr Youatt remarks, 'have been purely bred from the original stock
of Mr Bakewell for upwards of fifty years. There is not a suspicion existing in
the mind of any one at all acquainted with the subject that the owner of either
of them has deviated in any one instance from the pure blood of Mr Bakewell's
flock, and yet the difference between the sheep possessed by these two
gentlemen is so great that they have the appearance of being quite different
varieties.'
If there exist savages so barbarous as
never to think of the inherited character of the offspring of their domestic
animals, yet any one animal particularly useful to them, for any special
purpose, would be carefully preserved during famines and other accidents, to
which savages are so liable, and such choice animals would thus generally leave
more offspring than the inferior ones; so that in this case there would be a
kind of unconscious selection going on. We see the value set on animals even by
the barbarians of Tierra del Fuego, by their killing and devouring their old
women, in times of dearth, as of less value than their dogs.
In plants the same gradual process of
improvement, through the occasional preservation of the best individuals,
whether or not sufficiently distinct to be ranked at their first appearance as
distinct varieties, and whether or not two or more species or races have become
blended together by crossing, may plainly be recognised in the increased size
and beauty which we now see in the varieties of the heartsease, rose,
pelargonium, dahlia, and other plants, when compared with the older varieties
or with their parent-stocks. No one would ever expect to get a first-rate
heartsease or dahlia from the seed of a wild plant. No one would expect to
raise a first-rate melting pear from the seed of a wild pear, though he might
succeed from a poor seedling growing wild, if it had come from a garden-stock.
The pear, though cultivated in classical times, appears, from Pliny's
description, to have been a fruit of very inferior quality. I have seen great
surprise expressed in horticultural works at the wonderful skill of gardeners,
in having produced such splendid results from such poor materials; but the art,
I cannot doubt, has been simple, and, as far as the final result is concerned,
has been followed almost unconsciously. It has consisted in always cultivating
the best known variety, sowing its seeds, and, when a slightly better variety
has chanced to appear, selecting it, and so onwards. But the gardeners of the
classical period, who cultivated the best pear they could procure, never
thought what splendid fruit we should eat; though we owe our excellent fruit,
in some small degree, to their having naturally chosen and preserved the best
varieties they could anywhere find.
A large amount of change in our cultivated
plants, thus slowly and unconsciously accumulated, explains, as I believe, the
well-known fact, that in a vast number of cases we cannot recognise, and
therefore do not know, the wild parent-stocks of the plants which have been
longest cultivated in our flower and kitchen gardens. If it has taken centuries
or thousands of years to improve or modify most of our plants up to their
present standard of usefulness to man, we can understand how it is that neither
Australia, the Cape of Good Hope, nor any other region inhabited by quite
uncivilised man, has afforded us a single plant worth culture. It is not that
these countries, so rich in species, do not by a strange chance possess the
aboriginal stocks of any useful plants, but that the native plants have not
been improved by continued selection up to a standard of perfection comparable
with that given to the plants in countries anciently civilised.
In regard to the domestic animals kept by
uncivilised man, it should not be overlooked that they almost always have to
struggle for their own food, at least during certain seasons. And in two
countries very differently circumstanced, individuals of the same species,
having slightly different constitutions or structure, would often succeed
better in the one country than in the other, and thus by a process of 'natural
selection,' as will hereafter be more fully explained, two sub-breeds might be
formed. This, perhaps, partly explains what has been remarked by some authors,
namely, that the varieties kept by savages have more of the character of
species than the varieties kept in civilised countries.
On the view here given of the
all-important part which selection by man has played, it becomes at once
obvious, how it is that our domestic races show adaptation in their structure
or in their habits to man's wants or fancies. We can, I think, further
understand the frequently abnormal character of our domestic races, and
likewise their differences being so great in external characters and relatively
so slight in internal parts or organs. Man can hardly select, or only with much
difficulty, any deviation of structure excepting such as is externally visible;
and indeed he rarely cares for what is internal. He can never act by selection,
excepting on variations which are first given to him in some slight degree by
nature. No man would ever try to make a fantail, till he saw a pigeon with a
tail developed in some slight degree in an unusual manner, or a pouter till he
saw a pigeon with a crop of somewhat unusual size; and the more abnormal or
unusual any character was when it first appeared, the more likely it would be
to catch his attention. But to use such an expression as trying to make a
fantail, is, I have no doubt, in most cases, utterly incorrect. The man who
first selected a pigeon with a slightly larger tail, never dreamed what the
descendants of that pigeon would become through long-continued, partly
unconscious and partly methodical selection. Perhaps the parent bird of all
fantails had only fourteen tail-feathers somewhat expanded, like the present
Java fantail, or like individuals of other and distinct breeds, in which as
many as seventeen tail-feathers have been counted. Perhaps the first
pouter-pigeon did not inflate its crop much more than the turbit now does the
upper part of its oesophagus, a habit which is disregarded by all fanciers, as
it is not one of the points of the breed.
Nor let it be thought that some great
deviation of structure would be necessary to catch the fancier's eye: he
perceives extremely small differences, and it is in human nature to value any
novelty, however slight, in one's own possession. Nor must the value which
would formerly be set on any slight differences in the individuals of the same
species, be judged of by the value which would now be set on them, after
several breeds have once fairly been established. Many slight differences
might, and indeed do now, arise amongst pigeons, which are rejected as faults
or deviations from the standard of perfection of each breed. The common goose
has not given rise to any marked varieties; hence the Thoulouse and the common
breed, which differ only in colour, that most fleeting of characters, have
lately been exhibited as distinct at our poultry-shows.
I think these views further explain what
has sometimes been noticed namely that we know nothing about the origin or
history of any of our domestic breeds. But, in fact, a breed, like a dialect of
a language, can hardly be said to have had a definite origin. A man preserves
and breeds from an individual with some slight deviation of structure, or takes
more care than usual in matching his best animals and thus improves them, and
the improved individuals slowly spread in the immediate neighbourhood. But as
yet they will hardly have a distinct name, and from being only slightly valued,
their history will be disregarded. When further improved by the same slow and
gradual process, they will spread more widely, and will get recognised as
something distinct and valuable, and will then probably first receive a
provincial name. In semi-civilised countries, with little free communication,
the spreading and knowledge of any new sub-breed will be a slow process. As
soon as the points of value of the new sub-breed are once fully acknowledged,
the principle, as I have called it, of unconscious selection will always tend,
perhaps more at one period than at another, as the breed rises or falls in
fashion, perhaps more in one district than in another, according to the state
of civilisation of the inhabitants slowly to add to the characteristic features
of the breed, whatever they may be. But the chance will be infinitely small of
any record having been preserved of such slow, varying, and insensible changes.
I must now say a few words on the
circumstances, favourable, or the reverse, to man's power of selection. A high
degree of variability is obviously favourable, as freely giving the materials
for selection to work on; not that mere individual differences are not amply
sufficient, with extreme care, to allow of the accumulation of a large amount
of modification in almost any desired direction. But as variations manifestly
useful or pleasing to man appear only occasionally, the chance of their
appearance will be much increased by a large number of individuals being kept;
and hence this comes to be of the highest importance to success. On this
principle Marshall has remarked, with respect to the sheep of parts of
Yorkshire, that 'as they generally belong to poor people, and are mostly in
small lots, they never can be improved.' On the other hand,
nurserymen, from raising large stocks of the same plants, are generally far
more successful than amateurs in getting new and valuable varieties. The
keeping of a large number of individuals of a species in any country requires
that the species should be placed under favourable conditions of life, so as to
breed freely in that country. When the individuals of any species are scanty,
all the individuals, whatever their quality may be, will generally be allowed
to breed, and this will effectually prevent selection. But probably the most
important point of all, is, that the animal or plant should be so highly useful
to man, or so much valued by him, that the closest attention should be paid to
even the slightest deviation in the qualities or structure of each individual.
Unless such attention be paid nothing can be effected. I have seen it gravely
remarked, that it was most fortunate that the strawberry began to vary just
when gardeners began to attend closely to this plant. No doubt the strawberry
had always varied since it was cultivated, but the slight varieties had been
neglected. As soon, however, as gardeners picked out individual plants with
slightly larger, earlier, or better fruit, and raised seedlings from them, and
again picked out the best seedlings and bred from them, then, there appeared
(aided by some crossing with distinct species) those many admirable varieties
of the strawberry which have been raised during the last thirty or forty years.
In the case of animals with separate
sexes, facility in preventing crosses is an important element of success in the
formation of new races, at least, in a country which is already stocked with
other races. In this respect enclosure of the land plays a part. Wandering
savages or the inhabitants of open plains rarely possess more than one breed of
the same species. Pigeons can be mated for life, and this is a great
convenience to the fancier, for thus many races may be kept true, though
mingled in the same aviary; and this circumstance must have largely favoured
the improvement and formation of new breeds. Pigeons, I may add, can be
propagated in great numbers and at a very quick rate, and inferior birds may be
freely rejected, as when killed they serve for food. On the other hand, cats,
from their nocturnal rambling habits, cannot be matched, and, although so much
valued by women and children, we hardly ever see a distinct breed kept up; such
breeds as we do sometimes see are almost always imported from some other
country, often from islands. Although I do not doubt that some domestic animals
vary less than others, yet the rarity or absence of distinct breeds of the cat,
the donkey, peacock, goose, &c., may be attributed in main part to
selection not having been brought into play: in cats, from the difficulty in
pairing them; in donkeys, from only a few being kept by poor people, and little
attention paid to their breeding; in peacocks, from not being very easily
reared and a large stock not kept; in geese, from being valuable only for two
purposes, food and feathers, and more especially from no pleasure having been
felt in the display of distinct breeds.
To sum up on the origin of our Domestic
Races of animals and plants. I believe that the conditions of life, from their
action on the reproductive system, are so far of the highest importance as
causing variability. I do not believe that variability is an inherent and
necessary contingency, under all circumstances, with all organic beings, as
some authors have thought. The effects of variability are modified by various
degrees of inheritance and of reversion. Variability is governed by many
unknown laws, more especially by that of correlation of growth. Something may
be attributed to the direct action of the conditions of life. Something must be
attributed to use and disuse. The final result is thus rendered infinitely
complex. In some cases, I do not doubt that the intercrossing of species,
aboriginally distinct, has played an important part in the origin of our
domestic productions. When in any country several domestic breeds have once
been established, their occasional intercrossing, with the aid of selection,
has, no doubt, largely aided in the formation of new sub-breeds; but the
importance of the crossing of varieties has, I believe, been greatly
exaggerated, both in regard to animals and to those plants which are propagated
by seed. In plants which are temporarily propagated by cuttings, buds, &c.,
the importance of the crossing both of distinct species and of varieties is
immense; for the cultivator here quite disregards the extreme variability both
of hybrids and mongrels, and the frequent sterility of hybrids; but the cases
of plants not propagated by seed are of little importance to us, for their
endurance is only temporary. Over all these causes of Change I am convinced
that the accumulative action of Selection, whether applied methodically and
more quickly, or unconsciously and more slowly, but more efficiently, is by far
the predominant power.
Tidak ada komentar:
Posting Komentar